New!Stay informed with our freeemail newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

When we see what others do, our brain sees not what we see, but what we expect

Date:
November 14, 2023
Source:
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience - KNAW
Summary:
When we engage in social interactions, like shaking hands or having a conversation, our observation of other people's actions is crucial. But what exactly happens in our brain during this process: how do the different brain regions talk to each other? Researchers provide an intriguing answer: our perception of what others do depends more on what we expect to happen than previously believed.
Share:
FULL STORY

When we engage in social interactions, like shaking hands or having a conversation, our observation of other people's actions is crucial. But what exactly happens in our brain during this process: how do the different brain regions talk to each other? Researchers at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience provide an intriguing answer: our perception of what others do depends more on what we expect to happen than previously believed.

For some time, researchers have been trying to understand how our brains process other people's actions. It is known, for example, that watching someone perform an action activates similar brain areas compared to when we perform that action ourselves. People assumed these brain regions become activated in a particular order: seeing what others do first activates visual brain regions, then later, parietal and premotor regions we normally use to perform similar actions. Scientists thought that this flow of information, from our eyes to our own actions, is what makes us understand what others do. This belief is based on measurements of brain activity in humans and monkeys while they watched simple actions, such as picking up a knife, presented in isolation in the lab. In reality, actions don't usually happen in isolation, out of the blue: they follow a predictable sequence with an end-goal in mind, like making breakfast. How does our brain deal with this?

Chaoyi Qin, Frederic Michon and their colleagues, led by Christian Keysers and Valeria Gazzola provide us with an intriguing answer: if we observe actions in such meaningful sequences, our brains increasingly ignore what comes into our eyes, and depend more onpredictionsof whatshouldhappen next, derived from our own motor system. "What we would do next, becomes what our brain sees," summarizes Christian Keysers, a senior author of the study and director of the social brain lab in the institute. To arrive at that counterintuitive conclusion, the team, in collaboration with the Jichi Medical University in Japan, had the unique opportunity to measure brain activity directly from the brain of epilepsy patients who participated in intracranial eeg-research for medical purposes. Such an examination involves measuring the brain's electrical activity using electrodes that are not on the skull, but under it.

Unique opportunity

The advantage of this technique is that it is the only technique that allows to directly measure the electrical activity the brain uses to work. Clinically, it is used as a final step for medication-resistant epilepsy patients, as it can determine the exact source of epilepsy. But while the medical team waits for epileptic seizures to occur, these patients have a period in which they have to stay in their hospital bed and have nothing to do but wait -- researchers used this period as an opportunity to peak into the working of the brain with unprecedented temporal and spatial accuracy.

During the experiment, participants performed a simple task: they watched a video in which someone performed various daily actions, such as preparing breakfast or folding a shirt. During that time, their electrical brain activity could be measures through the implanted electrodes across the brain regions involved in action observation to examine how they talk to each other. Two different conditions were tested, resulting in differing brain activity while watching. In one, the video was shown -- as we would normally see the action unfold every morning -- in its natural sequence: you see someone pick a bread-roll, then a knife, then cut open the roll, then scoop some butter etc.; in the other, these individual acts were re-shuffled into a random order. People saw the exact same actions in the two conditions, but only in the natural order, can their brain utilize its knowledge of how it would butter a bread-roll to predict what action comes next.

Different flow of information

Using sophisticated analyses in collaboration with Pascal Fries of the Ernst Strüngmann Institute (ESI) in Germany, what the team could reveal is that when participants viewed the reshuffled, unpredictable sequence, the brain indeed had an information flow going from visual brain regions, thought to describe what the eye is seeing, to parietal and premotor regions, that also controls our own actions -- just as the classical model predicted. But when participants could view the natural sequences, the activity changed dramatically. "Now, information was actually flowing from the premotor regions, that know how we prepare breakfast ourselves, down to the parietal cortex, and suppressed activity in the visual cortex," explains Valeria Gazzola. "It is as if they stopped to see with their eyes, and started to see what they would have done themselves."

Their finding is part of wider realization in the neuroscience community, that our brain does not simply react to what comes in through our senses. Instead, we have a predictive brain, that permanently predicts what comes next. The expected sensory input is then suppressed. We see the world from the inside out, rather than from the outside in. Of course, if what we see violates our expectations, the expectation-driven suppression fails, and we become aware of what we actually see rather than what we expected to see.


Story Source:

Materialsprovided byNetherlands Institute for Neuroscience - KNAW.注意:内容可能被编辑风格d length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Chaoyi Qin, Frederic Michon, Yoshiyuki Onuki, Yohei Ishishita, Keisuke Otani, Kensuke Kawai, Pascal Fries, Valeria Gazzola, Christian Keysers.Predictability alters information flow during action observation in human electrocorticographic activity.Cell Reports, 2023; 42 (11): 113432 DOI:10.1016/j.celrep.2023.113432

Cite This Page:

Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience - KNAW. "When we see what others do, our brain sees not what we see, but what we expect." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 14 November 2023. /releases/2023/11/231114143711.htm>.
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience - KNAW. (2023, November 14). When we see what others do, our brain sees not what we see, but what we expect.ScienceDaily. Retrieved November 16, 2023 from www.koonmotors.com/releases/2023/11/231114143711.htm
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience - KNAW. "When we see what others do, our brain sees not what we see, but what we expect." ScienceDaily. www.koonmotors.com/releases/2023/11/231114143711.htm (accessed November 16, 2023).

Explore More
from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES