New!Stay informed with our freeemail newsletter.
advertisement
Science News
from research organizations

Hummingbirds' unique sideways flutter gets them through small apertures

High-speed video reveals strategies hummingbirds use to transit apertures too small for their wingspan

Date:
November 9, 2023
Source:
University of California - Berkeley
Summary:
Hummingbirds are highly maneuverable fliers, flitting forward and backward as they home in on flowers. But in the dense foliage many inhabit, they often encounter gaps that are too narrow for their wingspan. Since they can't bend their wings in flight, how do they get through? Researchers used high-speed cameras to capture their movements, discovering two unique strategies: they sidle through while fluttering, or fold their wings in a tuck and glide.
Share:
advertisement

FULL STORY

Most birds that flit through dense, leafy forests have a strategy for maneuvering through tight windows in the vegetation -- they bend their wings at the wrist or elbow and barrel through.

But hummingbirds can't bend their wing bones during flight, so how do they transit the gaps between leaves and tangled branches?

A study published today in theJournal of Experimental Biologyshows that hummingbirds have evolved their own unique strategies -- two of them, in fact. These strategies have not been reported before, likely because hummers maneuver too quickly for the human eye to see.

For slit-like gaps too narrow to accommodate their wingspan, they scooch sideways through the slit, flapping their wings continually so as not to lose height.

For smaller holes -- or if the birds are already familiar with what awaits them on the other side -- they tuck their wings and coast through, resuming flapping once clear.

"For us, going into the experiments, the tuck and glide would have been the default. How else could they get through?" said Robert Dudley, a professor of integrative biology at the University of California, Berkeley, and senior author of the paper. "This concept of sideways motion with a total mix-up of the wing kinematics is quite amazing -- it's a novel and unexpected method of aperture transit. They're changing the amplitude of the wing beats so that they're not dropping vertically when they do the sideways scooch."

Using the slower sideways scooch technique may allow birds to better assess upcoming obstacles and voids, thereby reducing the likelihood of collisions.

advertisement

"Learning more about how animals negotiate obstacles and other 'building-blocks' of the environment, such as wind gusts or turbulent regions, can improve our overall understanding of animal locomotion in complex environments," noted first author Marc Badger, who obtained his Ph..D from UC Berkeley in 2016. "We still don't know very much about how flight through clutter might be limited by geometric, aerodynamic, sensory, metabolic or structural processes. Even behavioral limitations could arise from longer-term effects, such as wear and tear on the body, as hinted at by the shift in aperture negotiation technique we observed in our study."

Understanding the strategies that birds use to maneuver through a cluttered environment may eventually help engineers design drones that better navigate complex environments, he noted.

"Current remote control quadrotors can outperform most birds in open space across most metrics of performance. So is there any reason to continue learning from nature?" said Badger. "Yes. I think it's in how animals interact with complex environments. If we put a bird's brain inside a quadrotor, would the cyborg bird or a normal bird be better at flying through a dense forest in the wind? There may be many sensory and physical advantages to flapping wings in turbulent or cluttered environments."

Obstacle course

To discover how hummingbirds -- in this case, four local Anna's hummingbirds (Calypte anna) -- slip through tiny openings, despite being unable to fold their wings, Badger and Dudley teamed up with UC Berkeley students Kathryn McClain, Ashley Smiley and Jessica Ye.

"We set up a two-sided flight arena and wondered how to train birds to fly through a 16-square- centimeter gap in the partition separating the two sides," Badger said, noting that the hummingbirds have a wingspan of about 12 centimeters (4 3/4 inches). "Then, Kathryn had the amazing idea to use alternating rewards."

That is, the team placed flower-shaped feeders containing a sip of sugar solution on both sides of the partition, but only remotely refilled the feeders after the bird had visited the opposite feeder. This encouraged the birds to continually flit between the two feeders through the aperture.

advertisement

The researchers then varied the shape of the aperture, from oval to circular, ranging in height, width and diameter, from 12 cm to 6 cm, and filmed the birds' maneuvers with high-speed cameras. Badger wrote a computer program to track the position of each bird's bill and wing tips as it approached and passed through the aperture.

They discovered that as the birds approached the aperture, they often hovered briefly to assess it before travelling through sideways, reaching forward with one wing while sweeping the second wing back, fluttering their wings to support their weight as they passed through the aperture. They then swiveled their wings forward to continue on their way.

"The thing is, they have to still maintain weight support, which is derived from both wings, and then control the horizontal thrust, which is pushing it forward. And they're doing this with the right and left wing doing very peculiar things," Dudley said. "Once again, this is just one more example of how, when pushed in some experimental situation, we can elicit control features that we don't see in just a standard hovering hummingbird."

艾尔ternatively, the birds swept their wings back and pinned them to their bodies, shooting through -- beak first, like a bullet -- before sweeping the wings forward and resuming flapping once safely through.

"They seem to do the faster method, the ballistic buzz-through, when they get more acquainted with the system," Dudley said.

Only when approaching the smallest apertures, which were half a wingspan wide, would the birds automatically resort to the tuck and glide, even though they were unfamiliar with the setup.

The team pointed out that only about 8% of the birds clipped their wings as they passed through the partition, although one experienced a major collision. Even then, the bird recovered quickly before successfully reattempting the maneuver and going on its way.

"The ability to pick among several obstacle negotiation strategies can allow animals to reliably squeeze through tight gaps and recover from mistakes," Badger noted.

Dudley hopes to conduct further experiments, perhaps with a sequence of different apertures, to determine how birds navigate multiple obstacles.

The work was funded primarily by a CiBER-IGERT grant from the National Science Foundation (DGE-0903711).


Story Source:

Materialsprovided byUniversity of California - Berkeley. Original written by Robert Sanders.Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Marc A. Badger, Kathryn McClain, Ashley Smiley, Jessica Ye, Robert Dudley.Sideways maneuvers enable narrow aperture negotiation by free-flying hummingbirds.Journal of Experimental Biology, 2023; 226 (21) DOI:10.1242/jeb.245643

Cite This Page:

University of California - Berkeley. "Hummingbirds' unique sideways flutter gets them through small apertures." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 9 November 2023. /releases/2023/11/231109221506.htm>.
University of California - Berkeley. (2023, November 9). Hummingbirds' unique sideways flutter gets them through small apertures.ScienceDaily. Retrieved November 12, 2023 from www.koonmotors.com/releases/2023/11/231109221506.htm
University of California - Berkeley. "Hummingbirds' unique sideways flutter gets them through small apertures." ScienceDaily. www.koonmotors.com/releases/2023/11/231109221506.htm (accessed November 12, 2023).

Explore More
from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES